Scientists hope to build better robots by studying the subtle movements of fish

Study sheds light on how animals use active sensing behaviors to navigate the world around them

Name
Jill Rosen
Email
jrosen@jhu.edu
Office phone
443-997-9906
Cell phone
443-547-8805

The constant movement of fish that seems random is actually precisely deployed to provide them at any moment with the best sensory feedback they need to navigate the world, Johns Hopkins University researchers have found.

The finding, published in the journal Current Biology, enhances our understanding of active sensing behaviors performed by all animals, including humans, such as whisking, touching, and sniffing. It also demonstrates how robots built with better sensors could interact with their environment more effectively.

"There's a saying in biology that when the world is still, you stop being able to sense it," says senior author Noah Cowan, a mechanical engineer and roboticist at Johns Hopkins. "You have to actively move to perceive your world. But what we found that wasn't known before is that animals constantly regulate these movements to optimize sensory input."

Animated gif of fishes swimming

Image caption: The study examined fish that generate a weak electric field around their bodies to help them with communication and navigation

Image credit: Noah Cowan

For humans, active sensing includes feeling around in the dark for the bathroom light switch, or bobbling an object up and down in our hands to figure out how much it weighs. We do these things almost unconsciously, and scientists have known little about how or why we adjust our movements in response to the sensory feedback we get from them.

To answer the question, Cowan and his colleagues studied fish that generate a weak electric field around their bodies to help them with communication and navigation. The team created an augmented reality for the fish so they could observe how fish movements changed as feedback from the environment changed.

Inside the tank, the weakly electric fish hovered within a tube where they wiggled back and forth constantly to maintain a steady level of sensory input about their surroundings. The researchers first changed the environment by moving the tube in a way that was synchronized with the fish's movement, making it harder for the fish to extract the same amount of information they had been receiving. Next, the researchers made the tube move in the direction opposite the fish's movement, making it easier for the fish. In each case, the fish immediately increased or decreased their swimming to make sure they were getting the same amount of information. They swam farther when the tube's movement gave them less sensory feedback, and they swam less when they could get could get more feedback with less effort. The findings were even more pronounced in the dark, when the fish had to lean even more on their electrosense.

"Their actions to perceive their world is under constant regulation," said Eric Fortune from the New Jersey Institute of Technology, a co-author on the study. "We think that's also true for humans."

Because Cowan is a roboticist and most of the authors on this team are engineers, they hope to use the biological insight to build robots with smarter sensors. Sensors are rarely a key part of robot design now, but these findings made Cowan realize they perhaps should be.

"Surprisingly, engineers don't typically design systems to operate this way," says Debojyoti Biswas, a graduate student at Johns Hopkins and the lead author of the study. "Knowing more about how these tiny movements work might offer new design strategies for our smart devices to sense the world."

Other authors include Luke A. Arend, a visiting undergraduate researcher at Johns Hopkins; Sarah A. Stamper, a postdoctoral fellow at Johns Hopkins; and Balázs P. Vágvölgyi, a Johns Hopkins associate research engineer.